Wednesday, December 19, 2012

CRPS: Spatial Perception (Where are my hand?)




Spatially defined modulation of skin temperature and hand ownership of both hands in patients with unilateral complex regional pain syndrome


  1. Gian Domenico Iannetti4
+Author Affiliations
  1. 1 The Sansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, Adelaide 5000, Australia
  2. 2 Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney, Australia
  3. 3 Faculty of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan 20126, Italy
  4. 4 Department of Neuroscience, Physiology and Pharmacology, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
  1. Correspondence to: Lorimer Moseley, Sansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, GPO Box 2471, Adelaide 5001, Australia E-mail: lorimer.moseley@gmail.com


Received March 21, 2012.
Revision received August 16, 2012.
Accepted September 19, 2012.

Summary

Numerous clinical conditions, including complex regional pain syndrome, are characterized by autonomic dysfunctions (e.g. altered thermoregulation, sometimes confined to a single limb), and disrupted cortical representation of the body and the surrounding space. The presence, in patients with complex regional pain syndrome, of a disruption in spatial perception, bodily ownership and thermoregulation led us to hypothesize that impaired spatial perception might result in a spatial-dependent modulation of thermoregulation and bodily ownership over the affected limb. In five experiments involving a total of 23 patients with complex regional pain syndrome of one arm and 10 healthy control subjects, we measured skin temperature of the hand with infrared thermal imaging, before and after experimental periods of either 9 or 10 min each, during which the hand was held on one or the other side of the body midline. Tactile processing was assessed by temporal order judgements of pairs of vibrotactile stimuli, delivered one to each hand. Pain and sense of ownership over the hand were assessed by self-report scales. Across experiments, when kept on its usual side of the body midline, the affected hand was 0.5 ± 0.3°C cooler than the healthy hand (P < 0.02 for all, a common finding in cold-type complex regional pain syndrome), and tactile stimuli delivered to the healthy hand were prioritized over those delivered to the affected hand. Simply crossing both hands over the midline resulted in (i) warming of the affected hand (the affected hand became 0.4 ± 0.3°C warmer than when it was in the uncrossed position; P = 0.01); (ii) cooling of the healthy hand (by 0.3 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.02); and (iii) reversal of the prioritization of tactile processing. When only the affected hand was crossed over the midline, it became warmer (by 0.5 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.01). When only the healthy hand was crossed over the midline, it became cooler (by 0.3 ± 0.3°C; P = 0.01). The temperature change of either hand was positively related to its distance from the body midline (pooled data: r = 0.76, P < 0.001). Crossing the affected hand over the body midline had small but significant effects on both spontaneous pain (which was reduced) and the sense of ownership over the hand (which was increased) (P < 0.04 for both). We conclude that impaired spatial perception modulated temperature of the limbs, tactile processing, spontaneous pain and the sense of ownership over the hands. These results show that complex regional pain syndrome involves more complex neurological dysfunction than has previously been considered.*


*over-used punchline!


Graphic from CRPS UK



       

Useful adjunct article published in same journal, September 14, 2009, available in entirety (pdf)

Space-based, but not arm-based, shift in tactile
processing in complex regional pain syndrome
and its relationship to cooling of the affected limb


G. Lorimer Moseley [1]

Alberto Gallace [2,3]

and Charles Spence [3]

1 PaiN Group & Department of Physiology, Anatomy & Genetics, University of Oxford, UK and Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute &
School of Medical Sciences, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
2 Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, P.zza dell’Ateneo Nuovo 1, 20126 Milano, Italy
3 Department of Experimental Psychology, Oxford University, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK

Correspondence to: G. Lorimer Moseley,
Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute,
Cnr Easy & Barker Streets,
Randwick, 2031,
Australia
E-mail: lorimer.moseley@gmail.com


Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) occurs after stroke, but most cases develop after peripheral trauma and without evidence of brain trauma. However, CRPS is associated with symptoms that appear similar to those observed in patients suffering from hemispatial neglect. Ten participants (four males) with CRPS of one arm performed temporal order judgements
of pairs of vibrotactile stimuli, one delivered to each hand, at one of 10 possible stimulus onset asynchronies, under two conditions: arms held each side of the midline and arms crossed over the midline. Participants released a foot switch to indicate which hand had been stimulated first. The order of conditions was randomized and the foot under which the switch was
positioned was counterbalanced. There were two blocks of 150 trials in each condition. The stimulus onset asynchronicity at which the participants were equally likely to select either hand, the point of subjective simultaneity (PSS), was compared between conditions and between those with left or right-sided symptoms. When arms were not crossed, the participants prioritized stimuli from the unaffected limb over those from the affected limb (mean SD PSS = 25 7.5 ms) and the magnitude of the PSS strongly related to the degree to which the affected hand was cooler than the unaffected hand (r = 0.942, P50.001). When the arms were crossed, the effect was reversed: the participants prioritized stimuli from the affected limb over those from the unaffected limb [PSS = –18 13 ms; main effect of condition F (1, 9) = 98.6, P50.001]. There was no effect of the side of
symptoms. These results show that CRPS is associated with a deficit in tactile processing that is defined by the space in which the affected limb normally resides, not by the affected limb itself, and which relates to the relative cooling of the affected limb. This pattern is consistent with data from those with hemispatial neglect after stroke and raises the possibility that chronic CRPS
involves a type of spatial neglect.

Abbreviations: CRPS = complex regional pain syndrome; JND = just noticeable difference; PSS = point of subjective simultan



References  (I post these whenever possible as they are invaluable guideposts for my own, much more laborious efforts to understand scientific texts,)

Acerra NE, Moseley GL. Dysynchiria: watching the mirror image of the
unaffected limb elicits pain on the affected side. Neurology 2005; 65:
751–3.

Acerra NE, Souvlis T, Moseley GL. Stroke, complex regional pain
syndrome and phantom limb pain: can commonalities direct future
management? Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine 2007; 39: 109–14.

Andersen RA, Snyder LH, Bradley DC, Xing J. Multimodal representation
of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements. 
Annual Review of Neuroscience 1997; 20: 303–30.

Andre JM, Martinet N, Paysant J, Beis JM, Le Chapelain L. Temporary
phantom limbs evoked by vestibular caloric stimulation in amputees.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology 2001;
14: 190–6.

Baylis G, Simon SL, Baylis LL, Rorden C. Visual extinction with double
simultaneous stimulation: what is simultaneous? Neuropsychologia
2002; 40: 1027–34.

Baylis GC, Simon-Dack SL, Greene K, Jelsone L, Rorden C. The effect of
ipsilesional cues on line-bisection errors: the importance of predictive
value. Neuropsychologia 2004; 42: 175–82.

Berberovic N, Pisella L, Morris AP, Mattingley JB. Prismatic adaptation
reduces biased temporal order judgements in spatial neglect.
Neuroreport 2004; 15: 1199–204.

Bisiach E, Bulgarelli C, Sterzi R, Vallar G. Line bisection and cognitive
plasticity of unilateral neglect of space. Brain Cogn 1983; 2: 32–8.

Bisiach E, Luzzatti C, Perani D. Unilateral Neglect, Representational
Schema and Consciousness. Brain 1979; 102: 609–18.

Bisiach E, Ricci R, Lai E, De Tanti A, Inzaghi MG. Unilateral neglect and
disambiguation of the Necker cube. Brain 1999; 122: 131–40.


Bisiach E, Vallar G. Unilateral neglect in humans. In: Boller F, Grafman J,
Rizzolatti G, editors. Handbook of neuropsychology. Amsterdam:
Elsevier Science; 2000. p. 459–502.

Bottini G, Bisiach E, Sterzi R, Vallar G. Feeling touches in someone else’s
hand. Neuroreport 2002; 13: 249–52.

Braus DF, Krauss JK, Strobel J. The shoulder-hand syndrome after
stroke—a prospective clinical-trial. Ann Neurol 1994; 36: 728–33.

Bruehl S, Harden RN, Galer BS, Saltz S, Bertram M, Backonja M, et al.
External validation of IASP diagnostic criteria for Complex Regional
Pain Syndrome and proposed research diagnostic criteria.
International Association for the Study of Pain. Pain 1999; 81: 147–54.

Cate A, Behrmann M. Spatial and temporal influences on extinction.
Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 2206–25.

Cohen HE, McCabe C, Hall J, Harris N, Blake D. Labile vasomotor
autonomic responses to ambiguous visual stimulation in patients with
complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) Type 1. Rheumatology 2008;
47: ii8.

Coslett HB. Evidence for a disturbance of the body schema in neglect.
Brain Cogn 1998; 37: 527–44.

Davis SW, Petrillo CR, Eichberg RD, Chu DS. Shoulder-hand syndrome in
a hemiplegic population – 5-year retrospective study. Arch Phys Med
Rehab 1977; 58: 353–6.

Drummond PD, Finch PM. Sensory changes in the forehead of patients
with complex regional pain syndrome. Pain 2006; 123: 83–9.

Eccleston C, Crombez G. Attention and pain: merging behavioural and
neuroscience investigations. Pain 2005; 113: 7–8.

Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F, Avanzi S, Ladavas E. Long-lasting
amelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation. Brain 2002;
125: 608–23.

Frettloh J, Huppe M, Maier C. Severity and specificity of neglect-like
symptoms in patients with complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS)
compared to chronic limb pain of other origins. Pain 2006; 124:
184–9.

Galer BS, Jensen M. Neglect-like symptoms in complex regional pain
syndrome: results of a self-administered survey. J Pain Symptom
Manage 1999; 18: 213–17.

Gallace A, Soto-Faraco S, Dalton P, Kreukniet B, Spence C. Response
requirements modulate tactile spatial congruencyeffects. Exp Brain Res
2008; 191: 171–186.

Gallace A, Spence C. The cognitive and neural correlates of ‘‘tactile
consciousness’’: a multisensory perspective. Conscious Cogn 2008;
17: 370–407.

Girardi M, McIntosh RD, Michel C, Vallar G, Rossetti Y. Sensorimotor
effects on central space representation: prism adaptation influences
haptic and visual representations in normal subjects.
Neuropsychologia 2004; 42: 1477–87.

Grande LA, Loeser JD, Ozuna J, Ashleigh A, Samii A. Complex regional
pain syndrome as a stress response. Pain 2004; 110: 495–8.

Grefkes C, Fink GR. The functional organization of the intraparietal
sulcus in humans and monkeys. J Anatomy 2005; 207: 3–17.

Guerrini C, Berlucchi G, Bricolo E, Aglioti SM. Temporal modulation of
spatial tactile extinction in right-brain-damaged patients. J Cogn
Neurosci 2003; 15: 523–36.

Haggard P, Wolpert DM. Disorders of body scheme. In: Freund HJ,
Jeannerod M, Hallett M, Leiguarda I, editors. Higher-order motor
disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. p. 261–71.

Halligan PW, Hunt M, Marshall JC, Wade DT. When seeing is
feeling: acquired synaesthesia or phantom touch? Neurocase 1996;
2: 21–9.

Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Wade DT. Left on the right – allochiria in a
case of left visuospatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psych 1992; 55:
717–19.

Hirsh IJ, Sherrick CE Jr. Perceived order in different sense modalities. J
Exp Psychol 1961; 62: 423–32.

Hudson ML, McCormick K, Zalucki N, Moseley GL. Expectation of pain
replicates the effect of pain in a hand laterality recognition task: bias in
information processing toward the painful side? Eur J Pain 2006; 10:
219–24


Iadarola MJ, Max MB, Berman KF, Byas-Smith MG, Coghill RC,
Gracely RH, et al. Unilateral decrease in thalamic activity observed
with positron emission tomography in patients with chronic neuropathic pain. Pain 1995; 63: 55–64.

Iwamura Y. Hierarchical somatosensory processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol
1998; 8: 522–8.

Iwamura Y, Iriki A, Tanaka M. Bilateral hand representation in the postcentral somatosensory cortex. Nature 1994; 369: 554–6.

Janig W, Baron R. Complex regional pain syndrome is a disease of the
central nervous system. Clin Auton Res 2002; 12: 150–64.

Karnath HO, Christ K, Hartje W. Decrease of contralateral neglect
by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline.
Brain 1993; 116: 383–96.

Karnath HO, Zimmer U, Lewald J. Impaired perception of temporal order
in auditory extinction. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 1977–82.

Kitazawa S. Where conscious sensation takes place. Conscious Cogn
2002; 11: 475–7.

Lewis JS, Kersten P, McCabe CS, McPherson KM, Blake DR. Body
perception disturbance: a contribution to pain in complex regional
pain syndrome (CRPS). Pain 2007; 133: 111–19.

Loetscher T, Regard M, Brugger P. Misoplegia: a review of the literature
and a case without hemiplegia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psych 2006; 77:
1099–100.

Maihofner C, Baron R, DeCol R, Binder A, Birklein F, Deuschl G, et al.
The motor system shows adaptive changes in complex regional pain
syndrome. Brain 2007; 130: 2671–87.

Maihofner C, Handwerker HO, Neundorfer B, Birklein F. Patterns of
cortical reorganization in complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology
2003; 61: 1707–15.

Maihofner C, Handwerker HO, Neundorfer B, Birklein F. Cortical reorganization during recovery from complex regional pain syndrome.
Neurology 2004; 63: 693–701.

Marshall JC, Halligan PW. Line bisection in a case of visual neglect –
psychophysical studies with implications for theory. Cogn
Neuropsychol 1990; 7: 107–30.

McCormick K, Zalucki N, Hudson ML, Moseley GL. Faulty proprioceptive
information disrupts motor imagery: an experimental study. Aus J
Physiotherapy 2007; 53: 41–5.

Moseley GL. Why do people with CRPS1 take longer to recognise their
affected hand? Neurology 2004; 62: 2182–6.

Moseley GL, Olthof N, Venema A, Don S, Wijers M, Gallace A, et al.
Psychologically induced cooling of a specific body part caused by the
illusory ownership of an artificial counterpart. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2008a; 105: 13169–73.

Moseley GL, Parsons TJ, Spence C. Visual distortion of a limb modulates
the pain and swelling evoked by movement. Curr Biol 2008b; 18:
R1047–8.

Moseley GL, Sim DF, Henry ML, Souvlis T. Experimental hand pain
delays recognition of the contralateral hand – evidence that acute
and chronic pain have opposite effects on information processing?
Cogn Brain Res 2005; 25: 188–94.

Moseley GL, Wiech K. The effect of tactile discrimination training is
enhanced when patients watch the reflected image of their unaffected
limb during training. Pain 2009; 144: 314–9.

Moseley GL, Zalucki NM, Wiech K. Tactile discrimination, but not
tactile stimulation alone, reduces chronic limb pain. Pain 2008c; 137:
600–8.

Muggleton NG, Postma P, Moutsopoulou K, Nimmo-Smith I, Marcel A,
Walsh V. TMS over right posterior parietal cortex induces neglect in a
scene-based frame of reference. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 1222–9.

Necker L. Observationson some remarkable phenomena seen in
Switzerland; and on an optical phenomenon which occurs on viewing
a figure of a crystal or geometrical solid. Philos Mag Third Ser 1832; 1:
329–37.

Nico D. Effectiveness of sensory stimulation on tactile extinction. Exp
Brain Res 1999; 127: 75–82.

Parsons LM. Integrating cognitive psychology, neurology and neuroimaging. Acta Psychol 2001; 107: 155–81.


Petchkrua W, Weiss DJ, Patel RR. Reassessment of the incidence
of complex regional pain syndrome type 1 following stroke.
Neurorehab Neural Repair 2000; 14: 59–63.

Pleger B, Ragert P, Schwenkreis P, Forster AF, Wilimzig C, Dinse H, et al.
Patterns of cortical reorganization parallel impaired tactile discrimination and pain intensity in complex regional pain syndrome.
Neuroimage 2006; 32: 503–10.

Rommel O, Gehling M, Dertwinkel R, Witscher K, Zenz M, Malin JP,
et al. Hemisensory impairment in patients with complex regional pain
syndrome. Pain 1999; 80: 95–101.

Rorden C, Mattingley JB, Karnath HO, Driver J. Visual extinction and
prior entry: impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion
perception after unilateral parietal damage. Neuropsychologia 1997;
35: 421–33.

Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farne A, Li L, Boisson D, et al. Prism
adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature 1998; 395: 166–9.

Rubens AB. Caloric stimulation and unilateral visual neglect. Neurology
1985; 35: 1019–24.

Schwoebel J, Coslett HB, Bradt J, Friedman R, Dileo C. Pain and the body
schema: effects of pain severity on mental representations of movement. Neurology 2002; 59: 775–7.

Schwoebel J, Friedman R, Duda N, Coslett HB. Pain and the body
schema: evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of
movement. Brain 2001; 124: 2098–104.

Shore DI, Spry E, Spence C. Confusing the mind by crossing the hands.
Cogn Brain Res 2002; 14: 153–63.

Sinnett S, Juncadella M, Rafal R, Azanon E, Soto-Faraco S. A
dissociation between visual and auditory hemi-inattention: evidence
from temporal order judgements. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:
552–60.

Spence C. Prior entry: Attention and temporal perception. In: Nobre A,
Coull J, editors. Attention and time. Oxford: Oxford University Press;
2009.

Spence C, Shore DI, Klein RM. Multisensory prior entry. J Exp Psychol
Gen 2001; 130: 799–832.

Sumitani M, Rossetti Y, Shibata M, Matsuda Y, Sakaue G, Inoue T, et al.
Prism adaptation to optical deviation alleviates pathologic pain.
Neurology 2007a; 68: 128–33.

Sumitani M, Shibata M, Iwakura T, Matsuda Y, Sakaue G, Inoue T, et al.
Pathologic pain distorts visuospatial perception. Neurology 2007b; 68:
152–4.

Vallar G. Extrapersonal visual unilateral spatial neglect and its neuroanatomy. Neuroimage 2001; 14: S52–8.

Vallar G, Antonucci G, Guariglia C, Pizzamiglio L. Deficits of position
sense, unilateral neglect and optokinetic stimulation.
Neuropsychologia 1993; 31: 1191–200.

Vallar G, Ronchi R. Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of
the neuropyshological literature. Exp Brain Res; 192: 533–51.

Vallar G, Rusconi ML, Barozzi S, Bernardini B, Ovadia D, Papagno C,
et al. Improvement of left visuospatial hemineglect by left-sided
transcutaneous electrical-stimulation. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:
73–82.

Vallar G, Sandroni P, Rusconi ML, Barbieri S. Hemianopia, hemianesthesia, and spatial neglect: a study with evoked potentials. Neurology
1991; 41: 1918–22.

Van Damme S, Gallace A, Spence C, Crombez G, Moseley GL. Does the
sight of physical threat induce a tactile processing bias? Modalityspecific attentional facilitation induced by viewing threatening pictures.
Brain Res 2009; 1253: 100–6.

Van Damme S, Legrain V, Vogt J, Crombez G. Keeping pain in mind: a
motivational account of attention to pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev;
in press.

Vibell J, Klinge C, Zampini M, Spence C, Nobre AC. Temporal order is
coded temporally in the brain: Early event-related
potential latency shifts underlying prior entry in a cross-modal temporal order judgment task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007;
19: 109–20.


Vongiesen HJ, Schlaug G, Steinmetz H, Benecke R, Freund HJ, Seitz RJ.
Cerebral network underlying unilateral motor neglect – evidence from
positron emission tomography. J Neurol Sci 1994; 125: 29–38.

Walker R, Findlay JM, Young AW, Welch J. Disentangling neglect and
hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29: 1019–27.


Yamamoto S, Kitazawa S. Reversal of subjective temporal order due to
arm crossing. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 759–65.

Zampini M, Bird KS, Bentley DE, Watson A, Barrett G, Jones AK, et al.
Prior entry’ for pain: attention speeds the perceptual processing of
painful stimuli. Neurosci Lett 2007; 414: 75–9






No comments:

Post a Comment

The Haddock Corporation's newest dictate: Anonymous comments are no longer allowed. It is easy enough to register and just takes a moment. We look forward to hearing from you non-bots and non-spammers!