Saturday, April 13, 2013

Farewell, Farewell -- Mary Black






Farewell, farewell to you who would hear
You lonely travellers all
The cold north wind will blow again
The winding road does call

And will you never return to see
Your bruised and beaten sons?
"Oh, I would, I would, if welcome I were
For they loathe me, every one"

And will you never cut the cloth
Or drink the light to be?
And can you never swear a year
To any one of we?

"No, I will never cut the cloth
Or drink the light to be
But I'll swear a year to one who lies
Asleep along side of me"

Farewell, farewell to you who would hear
You lonely travellers all
The cold north wind will blow again
The winding road does call



shrike556 flickr

The melody of "Farewell, Farewell" comes from the traditional folk song "Willie O'Winsbury". Richard Thompson wrote new words for it and recorded it as "Farewell, Farewell" on the Fairport Convention album "Liege and Lief" with vocals by Sandy Denny.

Thursday, April 11, 2013

CRPS-Related Dystonia


Integration of Sensory Force Feedback Is Disturbed in CRPS-Related Dystonia

Jonathan A. Coles, Editor

Abstract

Complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) is characterized by pain and disturbed blood flow, temperature regulation and motor control. Approximately 25% of cases develop fixed dystonia. The origin of this movement disorder is poorly understood, although recent insights suggest involvement of disturbed force feedback. Assessment of sensorimotor integration may provide insight into the pathophysiology of fixed dystonia. Sensory weighting is the process of integrating and weighting sensory feedback channels in the central nervous system to improve the state estimate. It was hypothesized that patients with CRPS-related dystonia bias sensory weighting of force and position toward position due to the unreliability of force feedback. The current study provides experimental evidence for dysfunctional sensory integration in fixed dystonia, showing that CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia weight force and position feedback differently than controls do. The study shows reduced force feedback weights in CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia, making it the first to demonstrate disturbed integration of force feedback in fixed dystonia, an important step towards understanding the pathophysiology of fixed dystonia.

Introduction

Humans use proprioception, vision and the sense of touch to effectively handle objects with a wide range of mechanical properties. Sensory feedback is noisy and has limited accuracy . In the central nervous system the sensory feedback channels are integrated and weighted to improve the state estimate. For example, during balance control, the relative weights of the sensory inputs from the vestibular system, mechanoreceptors and vision  shift with environmental properties, i.e., sensory reweighting . To effectively weight sensory feedback channels, an estimate of their accuracy is required. Bayesian inference has been suggested to underlie sensory weighting . Similar sensory weighting occurs between force and position within the proprioceptive system. Object stiffness, the physical relationship between position and force, allows translation from one modality into the other. When handling stiff objects like a cup, deflections are negligible so position holds no information on the applied force. However, when handling soft objects like a sponge, deflections are large and allow position feedback to contribute to the estimated force and vice versa. When stiffness is known, combining the sensory feedback of these two modalities (sensory integration) provides increased accuracy of the estimate of either force or position . Position feedback is weighted heavier on soft objects (large deflections), while force feedback is weighted heavier on stiff objects (small deflections) in healthy subjects.
Complex Regional Pain Syndrome (CRPS) is characterized by persistent pain, autonomic and trophic features  and is commonly preceded by a minor to severe trauma to a limb in the absence of an obvious nerve lesion . About 25% of the CRPS-patients develop fixed dystonia featuring abnormal postures and sustained muscle contractions, of which the underlying cause is unknown . Dysfunctional sensorimotor integration has been suggested to play a role in the pathogenesis of dystonia. The fact that many forms of focal dystonia can be relieved by “sensory tricks” is a strong indicator that sensory information is an important factor in focal dystonia . In addition, several studies specifically report dysregulation of force in dystonia. Recent modeling studies on the pathophysiology of fixed dystonia support involvement of force dysregulation as the computational neuromuscular model explained all defined features of fixed dystonia only with disturbed force feedback. Experimentally, the force variance during isometric force tasks increased in subjects with childhood dystonia due to cerebral palsy . Moreover, impairment of the ability to rapidly generate force and to voluntarily relax in patients with focal hand dystonia has been suggested to be related to down-regulation of sensory input . Problems to grasp and manipulate objects is a frequently encountered phenomenon in movement disorders . For example, patients with writer's cramp have increased grip force when lifting an object . Grip force adapts with sensory feedback suggesting that inaccurate grip force scaling is a manifestation of impaired sensorimotor integration. To study sensorimotor integration within the proprioceptive system in fixed dystonia we used a target matching paradigm where force and position were related using a (virtual) spring. If indeed sensory force feedback is unreliable, than Bayesian inference would dictate patients with fixed dystonia to reweight force and position feedback, favoring position feedback.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

After providing written informed consent, twenty volunteers – 10 CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia and 10 healthy controls – participated in the study that was approved by the medical ethics committee of the Leiden University Medical Center. The controls and patients were matched for age and gender (patients' mean age: 50.3 years (SD 10.3); controls' mean age: 50.8 years (SD 10.5); 1 male and 9 female in each group) and had equal handedness distribution (7 right-handed and 3 left-handed). All patients diagnosed with CRPS and dystonia were recruited in the Leiden University Medical Center (LUMC) and fulfilled the criteria for CRPS-I of the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) for at least one upper extremity .

Approach

We used a novel force-matching task that enables quantification of the sensory weighting of force and position . Subjects held the handle of a linear haptic manipulator (Fig. 1) with their dominant hand (controls) or the affected hand (patients; in case of two affected arms the dominant one was used). The arm and handle were blocked from vision to exclude undesired visual feedback. The manipulator simulated a spring and switched between two spring models:
Figure 1
Experimental setup.
  1. linear spring with stiffness An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e001.jpg(An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e002.jpg): exerting force An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e003.jpg, at position An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e004.jpg, according to Eq. 1:
    equation image
    (1)
  2. non-linear spring which exerts force Ftarget+An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e006.jpg at the position where the linear spring would have exerted the target force An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e007.jpg:
    equation image
    (2)
The experimental protocol, with target force An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e009.jpg and An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e010.jpg, consisted of four blocks of trials with spring stiffnesses (An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0060293.e011.jpg). The order of the blocks was randomized and with every new block the subject performed 15 training trials with onscreen visual feedback of the force which enabled the subject to learn the task and familiarize with the stiffness. The spring relates force and position such that they can be integrated by the CNS to get an estimate of either one. The subject was instructed to operate a foot switch when an indicator bar, representing the exerted force, was aligned with the target indicators. Pressing the foot switch triggered a force measurement of 0.6 second at a sample rate of 250 Hz. After each measurement, the subject was instructed on-screen to return to the starting position, i.e., the zero-length of the spring. The next trial was automatically initiated when the subject had crossed the starting position which was indicated by the appearance of the instruction for the next trial. After training, the subject performed a series of trials composed of three trial types:
  1. Reference trials where the subject was instructed to apply the indicated force using the onscreen indicators, exactly as in training trials.
  2. Blind trials where the subject was instructed to reproduce the trained force blindly and to operate the foot switch when (s)he thought (s)he attained the trained force.
  3. Catch trials were blind trials where the linear spring was covertly replaced by the non-linear spring. The spring model was always substituted at the zero-length position, to prevent the subject from noticing any change in force.
Blind/catch trials were alternated with reference trials to prevent drift from the trained force. On average, one in three blind trials was randomly replaced by a catch trial, effectively providing one catch trial every six trials. A total of 12 catch trials were recorded per block.
The difference in force (ΔF) between the blind trials and the catch trials revealed the sensory weighting between force and position feedback. The disparity in the spring environment allows the separate weights of force and position feedback to be determined, because in the catch trials force feedback biases the exerted force toward the trained force and position feedback toward the trained position.

Data analysis

For every trial, the measured force was averaged over the 0.6-second measurements. To prevent bias to the data due to accidental presses of the foot switch all trials with an average force of less than 5.0N were ignored. Subsequently, the force during reference, blind and catch trials were averaged over the repetitions. ANOVA's were performed to test for an effect of group on the force difference between the blind and the catch trials (ΔF) and the sensory weights. Effects of stiffness on the force exerted during reference, blind and catch trials and on the force difference between the blind and the catch trials (ΔF) as well as on the sensory weights were tested for the CRPS-patients and controls separately. Post hoc tests with Bonferroni-correction for multiple comparisons were performed on trial type.
To compare sensor accuracy, paired t-tests between CRPS-patients and their age and sex-matched controls on the standard deviation of reproduced forces with an infinitely stiff spring, and the standard deviation of reproduced positions with a zero-stiffness spring were performed. An additional paired t-test compared the ratio of the standard deviations because according to maximum likelihood estimation the optimal weighting depends upon this ratio .

Results

Fig. 2 demonstrates that both controls and CRPS-patients closely approximated the target force of 10N during reference trials with small standard deviations. In the target matching task, reference trials were alternated with blind trials in which the target force was blindly reproduced by loading a linear spring. On occasion a blind trial was covertly replaced by a catch trial with a non-linear spring revealing the sensory weighting between force and position. A significant effect of group was found on the exerted force across all stiffnesses and trial types as well as an interaction effect of trial type*stiffness.  Significant effects of stiffness and trial type on the exerted force were found for both CRPS-patients and controls. Post hoc analysis revealed that both the CRPS-patients and the controls exerted a higher force during blind trials compared to reference trials, indicating that without visual feedback of the force the subjects underestimated the exerted force.  Additionally, the force during catch trials with the non-linear spring was slightly higher than during the blind trials with the linear spring, which was significant for controls.
Figure 2
The measured forces in the three trial types against spring stiffness.
Fig. 3 presents the measured difference in force between the blind and the catch trials (ΔF). The results show that the force difference was greater in CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia than in controls.

Discussion
Our findings suggest that CRPS-patients did not optimally weight the sensory inputs. Nevertheless, the CRPS-patients do show adaptation of the sensory weights with stiffness which suggests that sensorimotor integration is not dysfunctional. It seems reasonable to assume that the consistent bias toward position feedback is purposeful, indicative of reduced reliability of force feedback.
CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia produced higher forces in blind and catch trials than controls did, whereas even the controls produced high forces. In contrast to our previous study where the subjects were all male and substantially younger , here a scaling of the differences between the blind and catch trials was required to attain the sensory weights. We expect that the higher reproduction forces are due to the age difference, but cannot exclude gender as a factor since 9 out of 10 subjects were female (women are more predisposed to CRPS).
Certain aspects of our study correspond to previous studies on grip force adaptation with task-specific dystonia. Healthy subjects often overestimate the grip force required to lift a novel object and then adapt the force rapidly within the first three lifts . Interestingly, all subjects with task-specific dystonia showed this adaptation, but consistently applied higher force levels than the controls even after ten lifts when no further adaptation occurs . In addition, a previous study  showed that patients with dystonia have similar levels of force variability to that of controls at low force levels (25% of maximum voluntary contraction). This corresponds to our finding that force variability of patients was not significantly different from controls at the relatively low target force of 10N that we used.
In a previous study we have shown that in force matching tasks with known stiffness conditions, there is no difference between position and force tasks with respect to sensory weighting . To prevent unnecessary strain to the CRPS-patients in the current study only the force task was performed. Instructing a subject to reproduce either force or position can be interpreted as focusing the subject's attention to one of the two modalities, possibly weighting force feedback heavier during a force task and position feedback during a position task. Although literature has shown that attentional manipulation of a specific sensory modality does not influence the relative weighting of that modality , the potential bias due to the current instruction would be toward force feedback and not toward position feedback.
Here we show that CRPS-patients with fixed dystonia present significantly different reproduction forces and sensory weighting that is biased toward position feedback. Assessment of sensorimotor integration may provide insight into the pathophysiology of fixed dystonia. The current findings support involvement of disturbed force feedback in fixed dystonia.

Acknowledgments

This work was greatly enriched by interactions with J.J. van Hilten, who performed the patient inclusion from the Leiden University Medical Center. WM and AS conceived the experiments, WM collected the data with practical assistance of D.M. Rosenbrand. WM, FvdH and AS wrote the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The work was funded by Dutch Government grant BSIK03016 (http://www.agentschapnl.nl). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

1. Körding KP, Ku SP, Wolpert DM (2004) Bayesian integration in force estimationJ Neurophysiol92(5): 3161–5. [PubMed]
2. Körding KP, Wolpert DM (2006) Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor controlTrends Cogn Sci10(7): 319–26. [PubMed]
3. Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH (2004) Merging the senses into a robust perceptTrends Cogn Sci 8(4): 162–9. [PubMed]
4. Yuille A, Bülthoff HH (1996) Bayesian decision theory and psychophysics. In Perception as Bayesian Inference (Knill, D. and Richards, W., eds), pp. 123–161, Cambridge University Press.
5. Mahboobin A, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ (2005) Sensory re-weighting in human postural control during moving-scene perturbationsExp Brain Res 167: 260–267. [PubMed]
6. Van der Kooij H, Jacobs R, Koopman B, Van der Helm F (2001) An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance controlBiol Cyb 84: 103–115.[PubMed]
7. Zupan LH, Merfeld DM, Darlot C (2002) Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movementsBiol Cyb 86(3): 209–230. [PubMed]
8. Peterka RJ, Loughlin PJ (2004) Dynamic regulation of sensorimotor integration in human postural controlJ Neurophysiol 91(1): 410–23. [PubMed]
9. Mugge W, Schuurmans J, Schouten AC, Van der Helm FC (2009) Sensory weighting of force and position feedback in human motor control tasksJ Neurosci 29(17): 5476–82. [PubMed]
10. Schwartzman RJ, Kerrigan J (1990) The movement disorder of reflex sympathetic dystrophy.Neurology 40: 57–61. [PubMed]
11. Veldman PH, Reynen HM, Arntz IE, Goris RJ (1993) Signs and symptoms of reflex sympathetic dystrophy: prospective study of 829 patientsLancet 342: 1012–1016. [PubMed]
12. Allen G, Galer BS, Schwartz L (1999) Epidemiology of complex regional pain syndrome: a chart retrospective review of 134 patientsPain 9: 539–544. [PubMed]
13. Merskey H, Bogduk N (1994) Relatively generalized syndromes. In: Merskey H, Bogduk N, editors. Classification of chronic pain. Description of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Seattle: IASP Press: 40-3.
14. Van Hilten JJ, Geraedts EJ, Marinus J (2007) Peripheral trauma and movement disorders.Parkinsonism Relat Disord 13Suppl 3: S395–S399. [PubMed]
15. Abbruzzese G, Berardelli A (2003) Sensorimotor integration in movement disordersMovement Disord 18(3): 231–40. [PubMed]
16. Tinazzi M, Rosso T, Fiaschi A (2003) Role of the somatosensory system in primary dystonia.Movement Disord 18(6): 605–22. [PubMed]
17. Nowak DA, Hermsdörfer J (2006) Objective evaluation of manual performance deficits in neurological movement disordersBrain Res Rev 51(1): 108–24. [PubMed]
18. Mugge W, Munts AG, Schouten AC, Van der Helm FC (2012) Modeling movement disorders – CRPS-related dystonia explained by abnormal proprioceptive reflexesJ Biomech 45(1): 90–8.[PubMed]
19. Munts AG, Mugge W, Meurs TS, Schouten AC, Marinus J, et al. (2011) Fixed dystonia in complex regional pain syndrome: A descriptive and computational modeling approachBMC Neurol 11: 53.[PMC free article] [PubMed]
20. Chu WT, Sanger TD (2009) Force variability during isometric biceps contraction in children with secondary dystonia due to cerebral palsyMov Disord 24: 1299–305. [PubMed]
21. Prodoehl J, Corcos DM, Vaillancourt DE (2006) Effects of focal hand dystonia on visually guided and internally guided force controlJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77(8): 909–14. [PMC free article][PubMed]
22. Prodoehl J, MacKinnon CD, Comella CL, Corcos DM (2006) Rate of force production and relaxation is impaired in patients with focal hand dystoniaParkinsonism Relat Disord 12(6): 363–71.[PMC free article] [PubMed]
23. Hermsdörfer J, Hagl E, Nowak DA (2004) Deficits of anticipatory grip force control after damage to peripheral and central sensorimotor systemsHum Mov Sci 23: 627–646. [PubMed]
24. Nowak DA, Hermsdörfer J (2005) Grip force behavior during object manipulation in neurological disorders: toward an objective evaluation of manual performance deficitsMov Disord 20: 11–25.[PubMed]
25. Odergren T, Iwasaki N, Borg J, Forssberg H (1996) Impaired sensory–motor integration during grasping in writer's crampBrain 119: 569–583. [PubMed]
26. Serrien DJ, Burgunder JM, Wiesendanger M (2000) Disturbed sensorimotor processing during control of precision grip in patients with writer's cramp. Mov Disord 15: , 965–972. [PubMed]
27. Nowak DA, Rosenkranz K, Topka H, Rothwell JC (2005) Disturbances of grip force behavior in focal hand dystonia: evidence for a generalized impairment of sensory–motor integrationJ Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry 76: 953–959. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
28. Schenk T, Mai N (2001) Is writer's cramp caused by a deficit of sensorimotor integration. Exp Brain Res 136, 321–330. [PubMed]
29. Johansson RS (1996) Sensory control of dexterous manipulation in humans. In: Wing, A.M., Haggard, P., Flanagan, J.R. (Eds.), Hand and Brain. San Diego, Academic Press, pp. 381–414.
30. Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher and more slippery objectsExp Brain Res 56: 550–564. [PubMed]
31. Helbig HB, Ernst MO (2008) Visual-haptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific attentionJ Vis 8(1): 21.1–16. [PubMed]